Lähteet
- Asikainen, K. (2005). Säveljonon hallinta ja musiikin teoria-aineiden oppimisvaikeudet. Sibelius- Akatemian koulutusjulkaisuja 13. Helsinki: Sibelius-Akatemia.
- Atkinson, R. ja Shiffrin, R. (1968). Human memory: A proposed system and its control processes. Teoksessa K. Spence ja J. Spence (toim.) The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory. Vol. 2, 90–191. New York: Academic Press.
- Baddeley, A. (1986). Working memory. New York: Oxford University Press.
- Baddeley, A. (2000). The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(11), 417–423.
- Benjamin, W. E. (1984). A Theory of Musical Meter. Music Perception, 1, 355–413.
- Brown, H. (1988). The interplay of set content and temporal context in a functional theory of tonality perception. Music Perception, 5, 219–250.
- Brown, H. ja Butler, D. (1981). Diatonic trichords as minimal tonal cue-cells. In Theory Only, 5(6– 7), 37–55.
- Brown, H., Butler, D. ja Jones. M. R. (1994). Tonal hierarchies and temporal order. Music Perception, 11, 371–407.
- Browne, R. (1981). Tonal implications of the diatonic set. In Theory Only, 5(6–7), 3–21.
- Butler, D. (1989). Describing the perception of tonality in music: A critique of the tonal hierarchy theory and a proposal for a theory of intervallic rivalry. Music Perception, 6, 219–242.
- Butler, D. (1992). The musician’s guide to perception and cognition. New York: Schirmer.
- Caplin, W. (1983). Tonal function and metrical accent: A historical perspective. Music Theory Spectrum, 5, 1–14.
- Clarke, E. (1999). Rhythm and timing in music. Teoksessa D. Deutsch (toim.) The Psychology of Music, 2. painos, 478–479. San Diego, California: Academic Press.
- Clarke, E. ja Krumhansl, C. L. (1990). Perceiving Musical Time. Music Perception, 7(3), 213–252.
- Cone, E. T. (1968). Musical form and musical performance. New York, Norton.
- Desain, P. ja Honing, H. (1989). The quantization of musical time: A connectionist approach. Computer Music Journal, 13(3), 56–66.
- Duke, R. A. (1989). Musicians’ perception of beat in monotonic stimuli. Journal of Research in Music Education, 37(1), 61–71.
- Duke, R. A. ja Geringer, J. M. (1991). Performance of perceived beat in relation to age and music training. Journal of Research in Music Education, 39(1), 35–45.
- Ericsson, K. A. ja Kintsch, W. (1995). Long-term working memory. Psychological Review, 102, 211–245.
- Estes, W. K. (1972). An associative basis for coding and organization in memory. Teoksessa A. W. Melton ja E. Martin (toim.) Coding processes in human memory, 161–190. New York: Halsted.
- Garner, W. R., ja Gottwald, R. L. (1968). The perception and learning of temporal patterns. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 20, 97–109.
- Goldstone, R. L. (1994). The role of similarity in categorization: Providing a groundwork. Cognition, 52, 125–157.
- Hindemith, P. 1940. Unterweisung im Tonsatz. I. Theoretischer Teil. Mainz: B. Schott’s Söhne.
- Hirsh, I. J. (1959). Auditory perception of temporal order. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 31(6), 759–767.
- Jones, M. R. (1974). Cognitive representations of serial patterns. Teoksessa B.H. Kantowitz (toim.) Human information processing. New York: John Wiley & Sons.
- Jones, M. R. (1976). Time, our lost dimension: Toward a new theory of perception, attention, and memory. Psychological Review, 83(5), 323–355.
- Jones, M. R. (1981). Only time can tell: On the topology of mental space and time. Critical Inquiry, 7, 557–576.
- Jones, M. R. ja Boltz, M. (1989). Dynamic attending and responses to time. Psychological Review, 96(3), 459–491.
- Jones, M. R., Boltz, M. ja Kidd, G. 1(982). Controlled attending as a function of melodic and temporal context. Perception & Psychophysics, 32(3), 211–218.
- Kalakoski, V. (2007). Musiikki muistissa. Teoksessa J. Louhivuori ja S. Saarikallio (toim.) Johdatus Musiikkipsykologiaan. Jyväskylä: Eino Roiha –säätiö.
- Karma, K. (1985). Components of auditive structuring – Towards a theory of musical aptitude. Council for Research in Music Education, Bulletin, 82, 1–13.
- Krumhansl, C.L. (1979). The psychological representation of musical pitch in a tonal context. Cognitive Psychology, 11, 346–374.
- Krumhansl, C. L. (1990). Cognitive foundations of musical pitch. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
- Krumhansl, C. L., Bharucha, J. J. ja Castellano, M. A. (1982). Key distance effects on perceived harmonic structure in music. Perception & Psychophysics, 32, 96–108.
- Krumhansl, C. L. ja Jusczyk, P. W. (1990). Infant’s perception of phrase structure in music. Psychological Science, 1, 1–4.
- Krumhansl, C. L. ja Kessler, E. J. (1982). Tracing the dynamic changes in perceived tonal organization in a spatial representation of musical keys. Psychological Review, 89, 334–368
- Krumhansl. C. L. ja Shepard, R. (1979). Quantification of the hierarchy of tonal functions within a diatonic context. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 5, 579–594.
- Krumhansl. C. L. ja Schmuckler, M. A. (1986). Key-finding in music: An algorityhm based on pattern matching to tonal hierarchies. Paper presented at the Mathematical Psychology Meeting.
- Lerdahl, F. ja Jackendoff, R. (1983). A Generative Theory of Tonal Music. Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press.
- London, J. (2004). Hearing in Time. Psychological aspects of musical meter. New York: Oxford University Press.
- London, J., Cross, I. ja Himberg, T. (2006). The effect of tempo on the perception of anacruses. Teoksessa M. Baroni, A. R. Addessi, R. Caterina, M. Costa (toim.) Proceedings of the 9th International Conference on Music Perception & Cognition (ICMPC9), Bologna/Italy, August 2006.
- Longuet-Higgins, H. C. ja Lee, C. S. (1982). The perception of musical rhythms. Perception, 11, 115–128.
- McAdams, Stephen (1989). Psychological constraints on form-bearing dimensions in music. Contemporary Music Review, 4, 181–198.
- Miller, G. A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review, 63, 81–97.
- Münte, T. F., Nager, W., Beiss, T., Schroeder, C. ja Altenmüller, E. (2003). Specialization of the specialized: Electrophysiological investigations in professional musicians. The Neurosciences and Music. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 999, 131-139.
- Neath, I., ja Surprenant, A. M. (2003). Human memory. Toronto: Wadsworth.
- Parncutt, R. (1988). Revision of Terhardt’s psychoacoustical model of the root(s) of a musical chord. Music Perception, 6, 65–94.
- Parncutt, R. (1994). A perceptual model of pulse salience and metrical accent in musical rhythms. Music Perception, 11(4), 409–464.
- Povel, D-J. (1984). A theoretical framework for rhythm perception. Psychological Research, 45, 315–337.
- Povel, D-J. ja Okkerman, H. (1981). Accents in equitone sequences. Perception & Psychophysics, 6, 565–572.
- Rameau, J-P. (1722/1986). Traité de l’harmonie réduite à ses principes naturels. Genève: Slatkine Reprints.
- Repp, B. H. (2003). Rate limits in sensomotor synchronization with auditory and visual sequences: The synchronization treshold and the benefists and costs of interval subdivisions. Journal of motor behavior, 35(4), 355–370.
- Repp, B. H. ja London, J. M. (2005). Production and synchronization of uneven rhythms at fast tempi. Music Perception, 23(1), 61–78.
- Restle, F. (1970). Theory of serial pattern learning: Structural trees. Psychological Review, 77, 481–495.
- Riemann, H. (1898). Handbuch der Harmonielehre. 3. painos. Leipzig: Breitkopf & Härtel.
- Rips, L. J. (1989). Similarity, typicality, and categorization. Teoksessa S. Vosniadou ja A. Ortony (toim.), Similarity and analogical reasoning, 21–59. New York: Cambridge University Press.
- Roeder, J. (1995). A calculus of accent. Journal of Music Theory, 39.1, 1–46.
- Schachter, C. (1987). Rhythm and linear analysis: Aspects of metre. Teoksessa F. Salzer and C. Schachter (toim.) The Music Forum, 6.1, 1–59. New York, Columbia University Press.
- Schröger, E., Tervaniemi, M. ja Huotilainen, M. (2004). Bottom-up and top-down flows of information within auditory memory: Electrophysiological evidence. Teoksessa C. Kaernbach, E. Schröger ja H.J. Müller (toim.) Psychophysics beyond Sensation: Laws and Invariants of Human Cognition, 389–407. Hillsdale, New Jersey: Erlbaum.
- Simon, H. A., ja Kotovsky, K. (1963). Human acquisition of concepts for sequential patterns. Psychological Review, 79, 369–382.
- Simon, H. A., ja Sumner, K. (1968). Pattern in music. Teoksessa B. Kleinmuntz (toim.) Formal Representation of Human Thought, 219–250. New York: Wiley
- Sloboda, J. (1983). The communication of musical metre on piano performance. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 35A, 377–396.
- Snyder, J. S. ja Krumhansl, C. L. (2001). Tapping to ragtime: Cues to pulse finding. Music Perception, 18, 455– 489.
- Terhardt, E. (1982). Die psychoakustischen Grundlagen der musikalischen Akkordgrundtöne und deren algorithmische Bestimmung. Teoksessa C. Dahlhaus ja M. Krause (toim.) Tiefenstruktur der Musik, 23–50. Berlin: Technische Universität.
- Tillmann, B., J. J. Bharucha, J. J., ja E. Bigand (2000). Implicit learning of tonality: A selforganizing approach. Psychological Review, 107(4), 885–913.
- Toiviainen, P. ja Snyder, J. S. (2003). Tapping to Bach: Resonance-based modeling of pulse. Music Perception, 21, 43–80.
- Van Egmond, R. ja D. Butler (1997). Diatonic connotations of pitch-class sets. Music Perception, 15(1), 1–29.
- Vitz, P.C. ja Todd, T.C. (1969). A coded element model of the perceptual processing of sequential stimuli. Psychological Review, 76, 433-449.
- Vos, P. G., van Dijk, A. ja Schonmaker, L. (1994). Melodic cues for metre. Perception, 23, 965– 976.
- Warren, R. M. (1993). Perception of acoustic sequences: Global integration versus temporal resolution. Teoksessa S. McAdams ja E. Bigand (toim.) Thinking in Sound, 37–68. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Lopuksi |
Tulostettava versio |